Understanding the Relationship between Language and Gender
Author: Doan Thi Ngoc
Language is the most important communication tool for human
beings. It not only reflects the reality of the society, but also has various
functions to strengthen and maintain social existence. Given such a view,
language does mirror the gendered perspectives and can also impact and
contribute to changing people's perception of gender over time. Thus, for a
long time feminists and sociolinguists have shown interest in describing
the differences in language use between women and
men, and studies of the cultural roles ascribed to gender. However,
interest in language usage differences between the two sexes has a very long
tradition in attempting to explain the distinction between language and gender
and how language supports, enforces, and maintains attitudes about gender in
general and women in particular.
This paper attempts to discuss the following points: (1) a
brief overview of key sociolinguistic approaches; (2) some important issues on
the relationship between gender and language; (3) some explanations on the
differences between male and female speech such as: power in gender
relationships, community practice, conversational styles and strategies,
attitudes and prestige, and discrimination and sexist language practices; and
(4) the implications for language planning.
A brief overview of key language and gender
approaches
Since the 1960s, sociolinguists (e.g., Camerron,1995;
Eckert,1989; Holmes & Meyerrhoff, 1999; Labov,1994, 2001; Lakoff,1975;
Tannen,1990; Trugill,1975; Zimmerman & West,1975) have been exploring the
gendered dimension of language. These early works have assisted in
distinguishing different aspects of sex and gender. Although many
sociolinguistic researchers use gender and sex interchangeably, it is very
critical to understand that sex refers to biological features such as XX
chromosomes for females and XY chromosomes for males. Some studies claim that
the assumptions associated with characteristics for male as masculinity, or
likewise characteristics for females as femininity, are inaccurate. Such a
biological view on sexual character leads to reification (making real/concrete)
of male and female inequality in our society. This interpretation results in
numerous sociobiological claims relative to neurological factors about the
relationship of male and female speech behavior.
On the contrary, gender refers to cultural and social
attributes that have been acquired via the socialization process. It is up to
individuals to choose characteristics that they deem suitable for males and
females and employ them accordingly. According to Wardhaugh (2010), gender is
also a fact that we cannot avoid; it is part of the way in which societies are
formed around us (pp.334). Therefore, Cameron (2007), Coates (1986), Crawford
(1995), Eckert (1989), Tannen (1990), Holmes & Meyerhoff (1999), and other
scholars have considered gender as a social construct in the study of language
and gender and social sciences. Gender division is a fundamental aspect of
society, as it is deeply imbedded in social organization and taught to
individuals from early childhood to adulthood stages. However, numerous studies
argue that gender categories have changed throughout history and varied
depending on specific race, ethnicity, culture, religion, nationality, region,
and class (Labov, 1994, 2001; Lakoff, 1975, Wardhaugh, 2010). Eckert and
McConnell-Ginet claim that “The force of gender categories in society makes it
impossible for us to move through our lives in a non-gendered way and
impossible not to behave in a way that brings out gendered behavior in others
(2003, p. 50, as cited in Wardhaugh, 2010).
The sex and gender definitions and explanations put forth
the following key approaches on the relationship between language and gender:
A. The biological approach
The biological approach was the initial focus in the field
of language research in the 1960s. It accounted for the distinction between men
and women in speech behavior on lexical, phonological, and morphological forms.
For example, typical male voice characteristics are different from those of
typical female voice characteristics; women usually have a fundamental
voice-frequency nearly twice as high as those of men (LIN8015, 2012; Md Sohel
Rana & Osama Khalifa Moh, 2011).
B. The cultural approach
In numerous studies, such as those of Tannen’s (1990) and
Maltz and Borker’s (1982), two cultural approaches were examined. They
argued that males and females belong to their own sub-culture and thus use
language to maintain identity within their respective groups. Maltz and Borker
(1982) indicated that girls tend to engage in pairs or small groups. Their
speech aims to build an intimate friendship, loyalty, equality, support, a
win-win situation, and a lack of desire to be a leader (in fact they are
encouraged not to be). On the contrary, males choose to work in larger and
hierarchically organized groups. They are more concerned with power, status,
and control. They compete to draw participants’ attention and win games. They
enjoy showing skills, size, and ability and try to express their dominance even
when others speak. These cultural differences in the male and female groups
lead to variance in the ways in which they converse and convey ideas
(Coates, 1986; LIN8015, 2012; Md Sohel Rana & Osama Khalifa Moh, 2011).
C. The power and dominance approach
Several scholars, e.g., Spender, 1985; Zimmerman &
West, 1975; Coates, 1986; and O’Barr & Bowman, 1980, highlighted the power
and dominance approach. They claim that women in a patriarchal system have a
low social status and position; therefore, the employment of standard language
use aims to raise their self-esteem. The difference in an interaction approach
assumes as a fact that interactions between males and females are attempts
toward male domination, or social inequalities between males and females. This
approach allows for intepretations of communication problems between men
and women because of the unequal hierarchical statuses and gender roles held in
society. Women’s speech was considered unimportant; therefore, they used
linguistic forms that were associated with their low positions in society.
These forms included tag questions, question intonations, hedges, politeness
strategies, and others. Conversely, men’s speech, for example, became an
implicit tool of patriarchal power through conscious and less conscious
gender-role training where they learned to dominate a conversation through
interruptions, talk time, etc. (Spender, 1985; Tannen, 1990; Zimmerman &
West, 1975). Thus, the early deficit approach was changed to a dominance approach.
D. The social constructionist approach
Last but not least, the social constructionist approach has
been a particularly influential model in recent studies on language and gender
(Coates, 1986; Holmes & Meryerhoff, 1999). These studies explored not only
social constructs, the relationship between gender and other aspects of
identity, but also the magnitude of context in determining how individuals use
language (LIN8015, 2012; Md Sohel Rana & Osama Khalifa Moh, 2011; Spender,
1985).
Some sociolinguistic issues in the field of language
and gender
Is language sexist? Society’s distinction between men and
women is reflected in their language. It is realized that there is a specific
“language” that is used by men and women. If a male tends to speak the “language”
used by women, he is considered to be crossing the boundary and is orientated
by the opposite sex. That is the reason why it appears a statement such as
"I would describe her as handsome rather than beautiful" would be
considered crossing the boundary. Because, in the English language handsome is
used only to describe males and beautiful is used only to describe females. For
example, in Vietnamese the words such as willowy as ‘thuot tha’ and graceful as
‘duyen dang’ are only used to describe the beauty of women in their youth.
However, these words have a negative rhetorical nuance when used to describe
men (Lakoff, 1975; Nguyen, 1999; Spender, 1985).
The following issues were ascribed by Holmes (1998) that
- women and men develop different language use patterns,
- tend to focus on the affective functions of an
interaction more often than men,
- tend to use linguistic devices that stress
solidarity more often than men,
- tend to interact in ways that will maintain and
increase solidarity, while (especially in formal contexts) men tend to
interact in ways that will maintain and increase their power and status,
and,
- are stylistically more flexible than men.
These issues are still under
debate and have become promising topics for interdisciplinary explanations (as
cited in Wardhaugh, pp.342).
In addition, there are issues that have been raised and are
still in great need of language and gender research (Maltz and Borker,1982;
Tannen,1990). They include:
· the
influence of gender on perceptions of categories,
· the
influence of gender and sex on male and female conversational discourse,
· the
relationship between women’s speech patterns and cross-cultural communication,
and
· the
implication of community of practice (COP) and how COP illuminates male and
female speech patterns.
Claims of differences between male and female speech
are:
A. Power in gender relations
Numerous studies by DeFrancisco (1997); Herring (1992),
Lakoff (1975) and Tannen (1990) analyzed the complex relationships between
women and power through interactions. Lakoff explored a discourse analysis of
writing texts from three major American institutions: academia using
Schegloff’s claims on the appropriate way of treating gender in a conversation
analysis, and art from an Oleanna play that highlights the distribution of talk
in the controversial Mamet, and proper politics in the way print media
sexualizes, objectifies, and ridicules women in politics. Lakoff argues that
aspects of the disruption of conventional speech convey subordinated and
dominant ideologies. Herring presents gender issues in computer-mediated
communication on the internet and she stresses that issues of power
relationships emerge and reinforce norms of society at large and how women
place and express themselves in the virtual word. Others who have viewed
cross-gender communications (Spender, 1985, Zimmerman & West, 1975; Tannen,
1990, 1993, 1994, 1998) explained that male communication is the norm and that
males interrupt, challenge, and control more in conversations. Such
characteristics are not for women in communication. In other words, language
behavior posits males into a superior status and females in subservient
positions. Therefore, women have to adopt standard language forms as a means of
responding to, working with, and challenging authority.
B. Community of practice
Community of practice (COP) is defined as another important
variable in language and gender research. According to Eckert &
McConnell-Ginet (2003), COP is a group of individuals who share a profession
and come together through shared goals. The practice includes both global and
specific aspects of structure, discourse, and interaction patterns. Thus, it
extends the notion of a speech community. All practices belong to an identity
group that helps members learn how to modify and shape their linguistic and
other behaviors in a way that fits the perceptions of self and others.
Apparently, this framework assists sociolinguists to examine three crucial
dimensions: mutual engagement, joint negotiated enterprise, and a shared
repertoire of negotiable resources accumulated over time. Examples of COP are
research groups, sport-teams, and policy making groups (Holmes & Meyerhoff,
1999; Wardhaugh, 2010).
C. Conversational styles and strategies
Are women more stylish in their speech or do they use more
politeness strategies than do men? Lakoff (1975) on “Language and Woman’s
Place” explained the deficit positions of women in language use in comparison
to that of men in various ways: women are taught not to express themselves
strongly, encouraged to talk in an uncertain ways, use more polite and weaker
forms of directives while men’s speech is described as direct, forceful,
authoritative, and confident. She pointed out that women are more likely to use
too many qualifiers or intensifiers like: “I think that” or “very”; or empty
adjectives such as: divine, adorable; or among others tag questions like (isn’t
it?). She identified these features to reveal that the male dominant position in
society places greater emphasis on differences in power of speech between men
and women (as cited in Wardhaugh, 2010).
D. Attitudes and prestige
In his work on language, class, and gender, Peter Trudgill
(1972) had research participants, who were grouped according to social class
and sex, read a paragraph that consisted of words ending in “ing” sounds. He
found that male speakers tended to drop the final sound of “g” and pronounced
the endings as “in”. That is, they used low prestige pronunciation. On the
contrary, female speakers were more likely not to drop the final sound of
“g” and pronounced the word-ending as “ing”. That is, women tended to employ
the prestige pronunciation of certain speech sounds due to their
hyper-correctness. To support this analysis, Trudgill adopted this view of
‘covert and overt prestige’ from Labov. Labov defines covert prestige as a
low-prestige language that is used by traditional working class neighborhoods.
In certain groups, standard language is not desirable because of a powerful
in-group marker and the use of non-standard forms reflects the group identity
and solidarity. For men, they show lack of non-standard forms because men in
society are judged through their work and masculine values, so they employ the
standard forms of language due to the ‘covert prestige’. In contrast to covert
prestige, women are more likely to overstress the standard form due to ‘overt
prestige’ as they aspire toward a higher social class. Their speech style is
considered better or more hyper-correct because their social positions are
lower and less secure and they are judged by their language and appearance, so
they use more standard forms. But the explanation may be different if women
attain a more social status through education, work, and social changes (Md
Sohel Rana & Osama Khalifa Moh, 2011).
E. Discrimination and sexist language
practices
For years, feminists have argued that sexist language
exists in every culture. The use of the gendered title pronounced (Mrs or Miss)
reflects not only sexism but also ideas and expectations about gender roles,
career selection, and goals for males and females in society. Gender bias,
occuring consciously or unconsciously and which underlies sexist language, is
another common error. Speakers tend to assume that man is the norm and woman
the “other”. For example, “Each student does his own assignments” or
using the words “congressman, policeman, or mankind for all people”,
thus indicating a view of men as the first citizen regarding morality,
spirituality, intellectuality, and absolute to women. In this modern era this
may cause offence, therefore, these forms need to be changed (Lakoff, 1975;
Talbot, 1998).
F. Implications of language planning
Gender bias is reflected in language both structurally and
in informal communication. To facilitate gender equality in the family, in the
workplace and in society, it is important to create equality in language
through language planning. This is the reason why eliminating expression of
gender bias against women in language has rapidly become part of the planning
of language with the name: “feminist language reform”, "non-sexist
language reform”, "feminist linguistic intervention", "sexist
language reform", "feminist language planning”, "feminist
language policy", and "reform of gender-biased language” (Lakoff,
1975; Chesire & Trugill, 1998, Nguyen, 1999; Wardhaugh, 2010).
Modification and reform are two gendered language planning
strategies. Modification is to change the habit of using gender discrimination
in language. For example, by eliminating the use of “man” and replacing
the term with the generic:
person/people, individual(s), human(s), or human-being(s)
instead; and by avoiding sexism when formally addressing persons by employing
Ms instead of Miss or Mrs regardless of their marital
status solves the gender discrimination problem. Another strategy for language
reform includes replacing forms that disregard women by the use of
stereotypical roles, such as “chairman with chairperson, salesman with
salesperson, fireman with fighter-fighter”. Recently, there have emerged some
new gendered words: statesman and stateswoman, sportsman and sportswoman.
To create equality in the use of the two pronouns he
and she, some researchers suggest varying the use of male and female
pronouns or use replacing gendered pronouns with “we”. Thus, we can see the
purpose of language planning by targeting equal rights for women is to
gradually reduce the disregard for women in language through eliminating the
habit of using language that carries gender bias. Along with changing the
habit of using gender bias language, new expressions are created that avoid
gender bias. Finally, language modification must be carried out in both the
written and spoken forms, for example, through curriculum change, as well as in
textbooks, theses, and essays in educational school environments. The media
industry is also responsible for combatting sexism by eliminating gendered and
discriminatory language (Cheshire & Trugill, 1998; Nguyen, 1999; Wardhaugh,
2010).
Discussion and conclusion
This paper highlights several key approaches that numerous
studies have described and raises many issues on the differences between
language and gender that sociolinguists have been concerned about and have
continued to find explanations and evidence for their use. In addition to
analyzing a number of claims on the differences in speech behavior between
males and females and gender interactions in discourse, the implications for
language planning is evaluated. As such, the differences between language and
gender, how gender is constructed in social practice, how gender intertwines
with other social factors such as identity, age, education, community of
practice, race, social class, and sexual orientation have been considered an interesting
and fertile field of research for researchers during recent years and for years
to come (Eckert & McConnell-Ginet,1998).
References
Cameron, D. (1995). Verbal hygiene. London:
Routleedge.
Cameron, D. (2007). The myth of Mars and Venus.
Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-921447-6.
Cheshire, J. and Trudgill, P. (Eds.), (1998). The
sociolinguistics reader, vol. 2: Gender and discourse. London: Arnold.
Coates, J. (1986). Women, men and language: A
sociolinguistic account of gender differences in language. London:
Longman.
Coates, J. (1996). Women Talk: Conversation between
women friends. Oxford: Blackwell.
Crawford, M. (1995). Talking Differences: On Gender and
Language. London: Sage.
DeFrancisco, V. (1997). Gender, power and practice: Or,
Putting your money (and your research) where your mouth is. In Wardhaugh
(2010).
Eckert, P. (1989). The whole woman: Sex and gender
differences in variation. Language Variation and Change 1: 245-67.
Eckert, P. & McConnell-Ginet, S. (1992). Think
practically and look locally: Language and gender as community-based practice. Annual
Review of Anthropology, 21: 461-90.
Eckert, P. & McConnell-Ginet, S. (1998). Communities of
practice: Where language, gender, and power all live. In Wardhaugh, (2010).
Eckert, P. & McConnell-Ginet, S. (2003). Language
and gender. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Herring, Susan C. (1992). Gender and participation in
computer-mediated linguistic discourse. Washington, DC: ERIC Clearinghouse
on languages and linguistics document no. ED345552.
Holmes, J. (1998). Women’s talk: The question of
sociolinguistic universals. In Wardhaugh, (2010).
Holmes, J. and Meyerhoff, M. (1999). The Community of
practice: Theories and methodologies in language and gender research. Language
in Society, 28 (2). 173-183. doi: 10.1017/S004740459900202X
Labov, W. (1994). Principles of linguistic change, I:
Internal factors. Oxford: Blackwell.
Labov, W. (2001). Principles of linguistic change, II:
Social factors. Oxford: Blackwell.
Lakoff, R. (1975). Language and woman's place. New
York: Harper & Row.
LIN8015 Introduction to sociolinguistics: Introductory
book. (2012). University of Southern Queensland.
Maltz, D. N. and Borker, R. A. (1982). A cultural approach
to male female miscommunication. In John J. Gumperz (ed.) Language and
Social Identity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 196-216.
Md, Sohel. Ranai. & Osama, Khalifa. Moh. (2011). Sex as
an independent variable related to linguistics variables. Jazan University,
Kingdom of Saudi Arabia
Nguyen, V. K. (1999). Ung xu ngon ngu trong giao tiep
gia dinh nguoi Viet. Publisher: Van Hoa Thong tin.
O’Barr, W. & Bowman, A. (1980). Women’s language' or
'powerless language'. In McConnell-Ginet et al. (Eds.) Women and
languages in Literature and Society. pp. 93-110. New York: Praeger.
Spender, D. (1985). Man made language. 2nd edn.
London: Routledge & Kegan Pail.
Talbot, M. M. (1998). Language and Gender: an
introduction. Cambridge: Polity.
Tannen, D. (1990). You Just Don't Understand: Women and
Men in Conversation. New York: William Morrow.
Tannen, D. (1993). Gender and conversational
interaction. New York: Oxford University Press.
Tannen, D. (1994). Gender and discourse. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Tannen, D. (1998). Talk in the intimate relationship: His
and hers. Wardhaugh (2010).
Trudgill, P. (1972). Sex, covert prestige and linguistic
change in the urban British English of Norwich. Language in Society, 1: 179-95.
Wardhaugh, R. (2010). An Introduction to
sociolinguistics. Oxford: Blackwell.
Zimmerman, D. and West, C. (1975). Sex roles,
interruptions, and silences in conversation. In Barrie Thorne and Nancy Henley.
(Eds.) Language and Sex: Difference and Dominance. Rowley, MA: Newbury
House, pp. 105-29.